Improving Research Efficiency and Productivity by Conducting Multiple Studies Concurrently With Immersion Medical Training

Contact Our Team

For more information about how Halldale can add value to your marketing and promotional campaigns or to discuss event exhibitor and sponsorship opportunities, contact our team to find out more

 

The Americas -
holly.foster@halldale.com

Rest of World -
jeremy@halldale.com



Improving Research Efficiency and Productivity by Conducting Multiple Studies Concurrently With Immersion Medical Training. Brandon Noyes, Chris Calvano and Anthony LaPorta discuss immersive simulation training while conducting three distinct research studies.

A novel approach to training and research is provided in this study conducted by Brandon Noyes, Chris Calvano and Anthony LaPorta.

Research is vital to the improvement of tactical and medical equipment and training. By conducting immersion simulation training, multiple research avenues may be pursued simultaneously. Rocky Vista University has worked with Strategic Operations Inc., California Federal Fire Department, and First Light Corporation to conduct a week-long immersion surgical simulation training in San Diego, CA. The lifelike simulation, from point of injury through emergency room and operating room, allowed for absolute control of multiple variables while still allowing for the surprise elements of real life tragedy (Hunt et al., 2012). Three studies were conducted simultaneously over the course of the week: surgical competencies, stress adaptation as measured by salivary cortisol and amylase and tactical lighting in surgery.

Paradigm Shift in Surgical Training

Numerous studies report that the quality of surgical education has deteriorated over the last two decades for a variety of factors, including (1) the demise of the Halstead paradigm of residency structure; (2) medical errors (the Institute of Medicine 2000 report To Err is Human: Building a Safer Health System found that up to 98,000 people die each year in US hospitals from medical errors - updated in 2013, this figure could be as high as 440,000), effectively ending the “see one, do one” era of surgery training on a live human due to its inherent randomness and ethical issues; and (3) the mandated work-hour restrictions introduced in 2003 that resulted from the 1984 death of Libby Zion, a patient at a New York hospital, that raised an issue of resident exhaustion. As a result, surgical residents in general are getting less surgical experience, confidence levels are decreasing, certifying examination pass rates are falling, and the quality of patient outcomes is deteriorating. This gap in training adequacy is being filled, less than adequately, by activities such as fellowships and the rise in simulation as an even more critical part of medical training activities (Hoang et al., 2013 or 2014).

It is too late to fix the problem at the post graduate level. By then doctors have had to certify that they are confident and competent to perform a certain number of items defined in a surgical case list, before going out into the world.

Our goal is to introduce perfect practice - before medical students stand over a human with a scalpel. We wish to create a realistic, fully immersive surgical environment (including emergency room, operating room, and post-op) where repetitive, scenario driven training can be done in a challenging, often chaotic, but safe manner, on realistic human-worn standardized patient surgical simulators, where individual and team skills can be practiced, and where fully instrumented simulators allow for post-op assessment and debriefing (Lipshy and LaPorta 2013)(Figures 2 and 3).


Figure 2. Repair of an inferior vena cava injury being conducted during a surgical immersion simulation training exercise in 2014. (Photo: Dr. Anthony LaPorta)

Figure 3. Competency assessment of critical skills in second year medical students' under hyper realistic simulated conditions. (Photo: Dr. Anthony LaPorta)

If an image of fighter pilots in aerial combat training - integrating flight simulators and aircraft - comes to mind, the analogy is correct: there needs to be a Top Gun of surgery; and a simulation environment so realistic that a medical student's first surgical experience on a human should be with no less confidence than the last surgery on a realistic human patient simulator.

Since the end of the "see one, do one, teach one" era (the "Halstead method"), physicians in training have had significantly less hands-on experience. Success is often born of failure. Being allowed to make mistakes - to fail - is no longer permissible in surgical education, for obvious and good reasons: patient safety. But it has the unintended consequences of loss of long-term training.

The US military over the past few decades developed a system of training that recognized that only by being allowed to make mistakes - and learn from them - could true learning be achieved. The military created Combat Training Centers (CTCs) at Fort Irwin, California and Fort Polk, Louisiana for ground combat training (the Top Gun school for aviators serves the same purpose) based on five principals: (1) provide a realistic setting; (2) allow trainees to fail; (3) face a credible opponent; (4) have objective assessment of performance; and (5) allow trainees to remediate mistakes. While these principals may seem simple, and maybe even obvious, the CTC training system incorporating them sets the US military apart from all other militaries.

We have used the same scenarios in conjunction with the light and stress research to train 63 second year medical students to become proficient on dangerous emergency procedures before ever attempting to do so on a human. The results of post-immersion training competency evaluations on these procedures showed passing rates of nearly 100% in all areas (Figure 4). Due to variation in the testing format between the first year and the subsequent two years, only the last two years data are shown in Figure 4. An even more stunning statistic is that of cognitive knowledge. 61 of 63 passed the equivalent of the eight week surgical exam in only 4.5 days, a failure rate of 3%. These are the same two people who had the most technical failures, and who also failed the regular junior rotation in surgery.


Figure 4. Salviary alpha-amylase (sAA) levels were measured during day 1,2, and 3 on operating room (OR) and emergency room (ER) roles. Samples were collected pre- event, post- event, 20 minutes post- event, and 40 minutes post- event.

Measuring Habituation to Stress

During these scenarios, salivary alpha-amylase (sAA) and cortisol levels were measured over a week-long period to observe if immersion along with surgical training would result in habituation (Mueller et al., winter 2012). During the day 1 scenario, sAA reactivity and recovery were seen in both operating room (OR) and emergency room (ER) roles (Granger, 2014). When compared to day 2 and 3, day 1 baseline sAA levels were much lower. This data suggests that during succeeding scenarios, students mounted a higher parasympathetic response. During day 2 and 3 scenarios, OR roles showed higher sAA reactivity and levels as compared to ER roles (Figure 6). Cortisol levels on day 1 show a post-event spike which was anticipated due to normal physiologic response to stress. Day 2 and day 3 collections show higher baseline cortisol levels which may suggest an anticipatory response to the stress (Figure 7). Students were first exposed to a scenario the day before, so a higher cortisol baseline can be a proper ANS response in anticipation of subsequent scenarios. Cortisol levels in the OR role show a decreased level at post-event collection which suggests a lower stress response. This can be due to increased confidence and focus during subsequent scenarios. Lower levels seen in OR roles compared to ER may be due to the focus on surgical technique over emergency room training.


Figure 6. Surgery being conducted under red-green lighting during a power failure simulation at a surgical immersion simulation training in 2014. (Photo: Dr. Anthony LaPorta)

Figure 7. Survey question posted to participants' post-surgical immersion simulation training concerning light source preference. (Photo: Dr. Anthony LaPorta)

The data show that students were exhibiting autonomic nervous system (ANS) reactivity with minimal hypothalamic-pituitary axis (HPA) reactivity. This suggest that students were able to react to the challenges by increasing focus, attention, cognitive function, and executive function to successfully complete the tasks as opposed to becoming stressed and exhibiting increased negative effect.

A full and complete analysis of the salivary data is currently being conducted. It is interesting to note that although the hormones may not show as much habituation as was anticipated, student performance has increased (Mueller et al., summer 2012). This indicates that while the physiologic response may still be heightened throughout the course, outcomes as measured by student competency assessment are not negatively affected at all.

Lighting Dynamics in Emergent and Tactical Environments

Human vision has evolved to have the highest retinal sensitivity to light in the green (around 550 nm) wavelengths of the visible spectrum. Within the retina, specific light-sensing cells, called rods, are dedicated to dark vision while cones are programmed for color vision detection. The highest density of cones is in the macula, which comprises our best central vision and best visual acuity (20/20 or better). The retinal periphery is dominated by rods, which have a best acuity potential of about 20/200, but possess a much better ability to detect light versus the cones. Specifically, rods are insensitive to long wavelength visible light (red).

When dark-adapted, the Purkinje shift describes a change in sensitivity favoring blue light detection. However, there are times when we need both photopic (light sensitive) and scotopic (dark sensitive) retinal systems to be active. Tactical environments include aviation, marine, and nighttime field operations. In these cases it is key to preserve dark adaptation. For example, a rotary wing crewmember needs to scan the skies for incoming fire and still maintain the ability to read and complete medical procedures in the cabin. The advantages of red illumination are lost when the visual target is blood. While dim red lighting preserves dark adaptation (and thus facilitates reading by stimulating some cones/photopic vision), the ability to detect red blood is compromised.

While white light is best for both contrast sensitivity and best acuity, it also poses the risk of easy detection by enemy observers in tactical situations. Red, blue, and green single hue illumination has been used in various degrees of success to reduce the visual footprint of operators while still facilitating complex vision dependent tasks. We have proposed a rational illumination hue selection based upon the tactical environment and the desired task (Figure 1).

Previous studies and our current report support the use of blended red-green light for blood detection as a superior technique to white, blue or green (Calvano, Enzenauer, Eisnor, and LaPorta, 2013). Clinically, ophthalmologists employ “red-free” light (essentially green filter) when viewing the retina to enhance detection of small hemorrhages from diabetes and vascular disease. The addition of a specific red wavelength to the green allows enhanced contrast sensitivity, maximizing scoptoic and photopic vision and accurate task completion

As part of our recent tactical medical simulation event, a number of studies were conducted assessing visual needs for care providers. At the conclusion of the week-long event, an online survey was made available to participants. We present the most salient responses in this report.

Of 23 respondents, 16 stated that vision issues interfered with task completion versus seven who reported no vision related impedance of tasks studied. Similarly, 17 of 23 responded that lighting issues interfered with medical task success versus six who stated no difficulties. The survey then asked which light sources were preferred in the three main testing scenarios: 1) surgical suite simulation, 2) assessment of hemorrhage on multi-cam uniforms, and 3) assessment of specific color hues in controlled illumination simulators. Of 27 responses, with a p-value of less than 0.01, 16 preferred the red-green blended light in the operating room, while one had no preference, and one each preferred white and blue light, respectively (eight responders did not participate in that simulation, Figure 5). For the hemorrhage on multi-cam detection scenario, 7/25 responses did not participate in the study. 16 favored the red-green blend versus one each for red and blue and none for white light. Similarly, of 24 responses concerning the controlled illumination simulators (five non-participants) the majority (15) preferred red-green blended light against one for blue and three with no recollection of a preference.

Overall, these results support qualitatively that red-green blended light provides an ideal balance between preserving scotopic (night) vision while enhancing photopic acuity needed to rapidly and successfully detect hemorrhage in tactical settings.

Conclusion

Conducting multiple research studies concurrently with immersion training of medical personnel can help to improve medical care via a myriad of avenues. Examples of the efficiency of this method are: maximized cost-effectiveness, control of variables while still allowing for life-like human tragedy, and increasing exposure to real-life scenarios while maintaining a large safety margin.

About the Authors

Brandon Noyes is a third-year medical student at Rocky Vista University College of Osteopathic Medicine. He is a recipient of the Health Professions Scholarship Program from the Navy and is prior service enlisted active Army.

Major Calvano is currently the United States Army Reserve Consultant to the Surgeon General for Ophthalmology. His training includes a dual PhD in Pharmacology / Toxicology and an MD. His Ophthalmology residency was at the University of Texas Medical Branch and MD Anderson Cancer Center in Houston Texas. He holds a voluntary appointment as Assistant Professor of Medical Education: Ophthalmology at the University of Central Florida.

Caleb Hudspath is a third year medical student at Rocky Vista University College of Osteopathic Medicine. He is a recipient of the Health Professions Scholarship Program for the United States Army.

Dr. Anthony J. LaPorta, MD, FACS, USA (Retd.) is Professor of Surgery and Director of Rocky Vista University's Military Medicine Honors Program. An avid skier, Dr. LaPorta was the first American to run the Olympic torch for the 2010 Vancouver games.

References

  1. Calvano, C.J. MD; Enzenauer, R. MD; Eisnor, D.L.; LaPorta, A.J. MD.: “Tactical Lighting in Special Operations Medicine: Survey of Current Preferences” Journal of Special Operations Medicine. Winter 2013.
  2. Granger, Douglas; Personal communication, 2014.
  3. Hoang, Tuan MD CDR USN; Kang, J. MD LCDR USN; LaPorta, A.J. MD; Makler, Vyacheslav I.; Chalut, Carissa; “Filling in the Gaps of Pre-Deployment Fleet Surgical Team Training Using a Team-Centered Approach” Journal of Special Operations Medicine; Winter 2013.
  4. Hunt, B; LaPorta, A.J. MD; Rush, R. MD; Moloff, A. DO; Schoeff, J.E. MD; Tieman, M. MD; Lea, Mark MD; “New Methods of Early Surgical Training Using the Human Worn Partial Task Surgical Simulator in Scenario Based Stress Immersion Training” MedSim Magazine, 4th edition, issue 3-2012.
  5. Lipshy, Kenneth MD; LaPorta, A.J. MD; “Leadership Skills in the OR, Part III: Operating Room Crisis Management Leadership Training: Guidance for Surgical Team Education” Bulletin of the American College of Surgery, October 2013.
  6. Mueller;G.R.; Moloff, A. DO MPH; Wedmore, I. MD; Schoeff, J.E. MD; LaPorta, A.J. MD; Ret. “High Intensity Scenario Training of Military Medical Students to Increase Learning Capacity and Management of Stress Response” Journal of Special Operations Medicine 12, Ed 2, Summer 2012.

  1. Mueller, G. R. ; Hunt B; Rush R. MD; Moloff A. DO MPH; Schoeff J.E. MD; Wedmore Ian MD F; Schmid James PhD, PA; LaPorta J.A. MD; “Intensive Skills Week for Military Medical Students Increases Technical Proficiency, Confidence, and Skills to Minimize Negative Stress” Journal of Special Operations Medicine, Vol. 12 Ed 3, Winter 2012; 45 – 53.

Featured

More events

Related articles



More Features

More features